Yuankai He
The Anatomical and Functional Evolution of the Default Mode Network
COMPARATIVE NEUROSCIENCE
EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
ABSTRACT
The default mode network (DMN) in humans is characterised as a closely-interconnected neural network that shows task-related de-activation. Whilst DMN abnormalities are associated with various neuropsychological disorders in humans, preclinical studies are generally limited. However, initial progress into the anatomy of proposed DMN homologues in rodents and non-human primates (NHPs) show the existence of a closely connected network functionally distant from primary sensory/motor regions in all these species, with a similar anterior-posterior subdivision. Examining the body of work in different species, the author proposes possible evolutionary developments in the anatomy of DMN homologues, starting with expansion of the frontotemporal subnetwork, an apomorphy of primates, and then transitioning from the ventral/frontotemporal pathway to the dorsal/frontoparietal pathway. Many symptoms of DMN-related disorders, including the shifting of attention (deficit in ADHD), the generation of social expectation by mirroring other individuals (deficit in autism spectrum disorders [ASD]), and object-recognition memory decline during ageing (probably dementia-specific), are not only conserved in common NHP species, but also provide further support for the frontotemporal-to-frontoparietal transition within primate species. Thus, the combined analysis suggests a very optimistic perspective that, with careful task design, behavioural studies with NHP will shed more light on the establishment of DMN as a clinical target.
Keywords: default mode network, evolution, non-human primates, funcational magnetic resonance imaging, clinical translation
INTERDISCIPLINARY COMMENTARIES:
Psychology: Is the default mode network the locus of imagination and morality, and what adaptive functions do these psychological faculties serve that distinguish humans from other primates? (Selwood-Metcalfe, E.)
Yuankai He
Corpus Christi College, University of Cambridge
Volume 1, Issue 2, pp. 89–98
Received: November 14, 2022
Revision recieved: December 10, 2022
Accepted: December 28, 2022
Published: January 20, 2023
Preferred citation:
He, Y. (2023). The anatomical and functional evolution of the default mode network. Cambridge Journal of Human Behaviour, 1(2), 89–98. https://www.cjhumanbehaviour.com/ns0016
© Yuankai He. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License.
INTRODUCTION
Defying established beliefs that goal-directed behaviour is associated with regional brain activation, the default mode network (DMN) was identified as a widely distributed group of brain regions showing decreased activity during various types of attention-demanding, goal-directed behaviours. These areas include ventro- and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC and dmPFC), medial temporal cortex (MTC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and the adjacent temporoparietal junction (TPJ; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014; Raichle et al., 2001; Smallwood et al., 2021). Clinical correlates of DMN with depression (Yan et al., 2019), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Miller et al., 2017), Alzheimer’s disease (AD; Greicius et al., 2004), autism spectrum disorder (ASD; Padmanabhan et al., 2017), anxiety disorder (Zhao et al., 2007), and many other psychiatric conditions sparked wide-ranging interest in the functional role of the DMN in humans.
Many theories on the function of DMN have been proposed in light of its distinctive activation pattern, including maintenance of passive cognitive processes that are suppressed in active tasks (Shulman et al., 1997), interaction between self and non-self (Wen et al., 2020; Yeshurun et al., 2021), and various memory functions (Philippi et al., 2015; Yuan et al., 2021). Whilst these theories are supported by a wealth of neuroimaging data (including position electron tomography [PET] and functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI]) which establish robust brain activity-behaviour correlations, the establishment of DMN as a target of clinical interventions necessitates a causal link. With a limited range of strategies to study the anatomical and cellular mechanisms of DMN in humans, insights have to be drawn from animal-based studies. However, these are limited in at least two regards: anatomically, all component regions of the DMN are hotspots in cortical expansion during human evolution, limiting the anatomical translatability of animal studies (Fjell et al., 2015; Hill et al., 2010); functionally, complex behavioural correlates of the DMN in humans, like moral judgement (Chiong et al., 2013) and autobiographical memory (Wen et al., 2020), are difficult to observe with animals. Hence, an evolutionary perspective would facilitate the integration and clinical translation of the body of work on DMN. In particular, this review assesses evidence on the existence of DMN homologues in common model organisms, then compares between identified homologues to infer possible evolutionary developments in DMN connectivity, and finally attempts to relate the anatomical evolution of DMN to its functional evolution and thus to explore the possibility of establishing DMN as a clinical target.
IDENTIFYING ANIMAL HOMOLOGUES OF THE DMN
The human DMN was initially identified by task-related decreases in brain activity, with evidence showing higher resting-state blood flow in mPFC, PCC, and MTC, signalling higher resting-state activity (Raichle et al., 2001). However, only a few studies directly applied this criterion to animals to identify the DMN homologues. An initial exploratory study compared PET-based resting brain activities of human subjects and our closest living relative, the common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), showing highest resting brain activity in bilateral dmPFC, PCC, precuneus, and left TPJ in chimpanzees which largely overlapped with the regions of highest activity in humans, except for higher left-biased laterality and lower vmPFC activity in humans (Rilling et al., 2007). An extension to this study comparing chimpanzees’ brain activities at rest and during a working-memory image-matching task showed task-associated deactivation in bilateral vmPFC, PCC, precuneus, left TPJ, and more diffusely across the entire bilateral PFC (Barks et al., 2015). Since the lobular structure of the chimpanzee is highly similar to that of humans, this evidence overwhelmingly supports the existence of a DMN homologue in chimpanzees.
Initial findings with the chimpanzees inspired studies exploring DMN homologues in other non-human primate (NHP) species. Compared to chimpanzee studies, which are limited by the slower life cycle of chimpanzees and various other practical or ethical reasons, rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and, more recently, common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) are much more established model species for neurobiological research. Whilst homologues of all DMN component regions (vmPFC/dmPFC, PCC, and the adjacent precuneus, MTC/TPJ) are found in the macaque (Reveley et al., 2017), and these areas generally show increased resting activity (Kojima et al., 2009), regions of task-associated deactivation do not fully coincide with the human/chimpanzee counterparts. Combined results from working-memory, attention, visual, and auditory tasks show that posterior DMN areas in PCC, precuneus, and TPJ coincide with the task-negative activation pattern in humans, whereas dmPFC shows task-related deactivation to a much lesser extent, and in vmPFC only the most dorsal part of BA32 is reported to show significant task-related deactivation (Hayden et al., 2009; Kojima et al., 2009; Mantini et al., 2011). It should, however, be noted that none of the putative DMN homologues showed task-induced activation. Hence, despite the interspecific difference in task-related activity levels, the existence of a default mode network in macaques over PFC, TPJ, and PCC/precuneus is generally supported. Although studies of DMN in marmosets have only recently started, a similar quantitative—but not qualitative—anatomical gap can be inferred. Only one study, to the author’s knowledge, compared the brain activity of marmosets at rest and during a simple visual attention task (i.e., fixate on stimulus when it appears), showing task-associated deactivation of PCC, precuneus, and TPJ. Whereas no task-induced inhibition is observed in the PFC, it displayed task-neutral behaviour (i.e., no significant change in activity during tasks) similar to macaques (Liu et al., 2019). Since these areas are also homologous to the human brain cytoarchitectonically (Paxinos et al., 2012), the existence of a DMN in marmosets can be confirmed. However, more information is needed to find a potential homologue of the frontal part of DMN in marmosets. Overall, evidence supports the conservation of the posterior parts of DMN in NHPs, whereas the functional cross-species continuity of the frontal DMN components, though anatomically homologous to humans, needs to be further examined.
Compared to NHPs, rodent studies offer a wider range of genetic/molecular intervention methods and a faster lifecycle, enabling larger-scale studies. However, though some studies have shown deactivation of certain rodent brain regions in response to sensory stimulation, including the retrosplenial cortex (RSP, homologous to human PCC), and their disconnection from other brain regions in both anaesthetised and awake animals (Ferrier et al., 2020; J. Li et al., 2015), whether stimulations compare to cognitive tasks in humans still needs to be established. Instead, the identification of DMN homologues in rodents is mainly based on the resting-state functional connectivity of candidate brain regions (see next section; Hsu et al., 2016; Lu et al., 2012; Stafford et al., 2014; Upadhyay et al., 2011; N. Zhang et al., 2010). Main candidate brain regions from cytoarchitectonic mapping include vmPFC homologues (prelimbic, infralimbic, medial orbital, and cingulate cortices), PCC homologue (RSP), TPJ homologue (temporal association [TEA] and the hippocampal formation), visual (V1/V2), and posterior parietal (PPC) cortices (Paxinos & Franklin, 2004).
Connectivity patterns of the DMN
Although the cytoarchitectonic homologues of all DMN component regions exist in the model organisms, task-related activation patterns of these regions cast doubt on the extent to which the human DMN functions are represented in these model species. Hence, investigations into the connectivity patterns of the putative DMN homologues in different species could unravel the evolution of their functional roles, especially for the regions with discrepancies in task-related activity. One key piece of evidence is obtained from the raw resting-state fMRI signal, where co-activating regions of the brain show high temporal correlations of random resting-state fluctuations (Biswal et al., 1995). Hence, two key hypotheses can be formulated: (a) different DMN regions show high resting-state coherence with one another and low coherence with task-positive regions—as a result, all DMN regions should be included in a distinct cluster in resting-state correlations; (b) intrinsic organisation within the DMN can be revealed by identifying sub-clusters within the DMN. These hypotheses may be supported by task-based PET and fMRI studies (supporting functional connectivity), tractography, and anterograde/retrograde tracing studies (supporting anatomical connectivity).
Human DMN connectivity
Findings from the human DMN are consistent with both hypotheses and have thus identified two main sub-networks within the DMN. Greicius and colleagues (2003) first studied the human DMN analysing the resting-state functional connectivity (FC) seeded in PCC and vmPFC,[1] and found significant resting-state coherence within the DMN for both regions with significant anti-correlation with vlPFC/dlPFC, a known task-positive region (Desgranges et al., 1998). Similar coherence within the DMN is observed in studies using other data analysis approaches to resting-state fMRI data, including mixed-effect analysis, independent component analysis (Teipel et al., 2010; van Oort et al., 2014), and voxel-based clustering (Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009; Vatansever et al., 2015). A novel approach by Passow and colleagues (2015) utilising the resting fluctuations in metabolic activities measured by PET also produced a similar cluster covering DMN. Recent results also include the midline subcortical structures (caudate, central nuclei, and medial pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus) in the same cluster as DMN (J. Li et al., 2021), but further confirmation is needed to determine whether coherence of the resting-state PET signal implies functional co-activation. Overall, these results firmly establish the DMN as a closely interconnected network relatively functionally isolated from other cortical regions of the brain, confirming hypothesis (a).
Importantly, the same clustering approaches (Greicius et al., 2003; Passow et al., 2015) also showed that, whilst PCC has higher resting-state FC with the posterior sub-network of DMN (PCC/precuneus and TPJ), vmPFC is more strongly correlated with the anterior midline cortical regions (vmPFC, dmPFC, and ACC), outlining the two putative sub-networks which may be functionally different. The same two DMN sub-networks can be identified in the FC during fixation and a rapid-response visuomotor task, and this subdivision is functionally significant since the connectivity between these two subnetworks is reported to be associated with enhanced performance in the task (Vatansever et al., 2015). Thanks to recent advancements in high-resolution MRI, the structures of DMN subnetworks can be further confirmed by the tracking of axons with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), which infers the axon direction from the higher diffusivity of protons along than across the axonal membrane (Basser, 1995). Main white matter tracts within the DMN, identified by DTI, include a major posterior network connecting bilateral PCC, precuneus, medial, and lateral temporal lobe, the genu of corpus callosum connecting bilateral mPFC, and two major tracts (cingulum and superior fronto-occipital fasciculus [SFOF]) connecting the anterior and posterior parts of DMN (Teipel et al., 2010; Van Den Heuvel et al., 2009; van Oort et al., 2014). These results clearly support the subdivision of DMN into two bilateral sub-networks (hypothesis (b)) whose function will be further analysed in the next section.
DMN connectivity in NHPs
Since chimpanzees are not commonly used for preclinical research and have already shown highly similar task-induced activation patterns to humans, most studies of NHP DMN are devoted to the rhesus macaque. The first study to analyse the resting-state coherence of macaques on a network level used independent component analysis and identified four closely related clusters resembling the human DMN, including two overlapping clusters around the PCC, one around TPJ/MTC, and the other near medial parts of dlPFC (A8b/A46D, only slightly lateral to the human DMN; Hutchison et al., 2011). However, the PCC/precuneus cluster included the adjacent secondary somatosensory cortex (S2) and thus showed strong positive correlations with the task-positive primary somatosensory and primary motor cortices, a phenomenon incongruent with the findings with humans (Greicius et al., 2003; Passow et al., 2015). Conversely, though studies using the seed-based approach (Mantini et al., 2011; Margulies et al., 2009) and a novel diffusion embedding analysis strategy (Margulies et al., 2016) supported the general locations of DMN components, the clusters they identified are least connected to the primary areas. Compared to Hutchison and colleagues’ (2011) approach, the latter two studies excluded premotor and S2 cortices and included large parts of vmPFC, the resultant DMN homologue covering regions almost identical to that of humans. One factor for such divergent results might be the different pre-processing paradigms for raw fMRI data, which might affect the correlation between neighbouring brain regions. Despite these disparities, the agreement on the gross topography of the DMN, combined with its task-negative behaviour (Mantini et al., 2011), supports the anatomical homology between human and macaque DMNs. Findings from fluorescent neuron tracing studies further support the existence of a highly interconnected network amongst PCC, TPJ, precuneus, ACC, dlPFC (Parvizi et al., 2006), centrolateral and pulvinar nuclei of the thalamus (Gamberini et al., 2020) provide even stronger support for the existence of an independent DMN homologue (hypothesis (a)) in macaques with similar subcortical projections, although projections from the posterolateral thalamus nucleus to the DMN are not matched in humans.
The internal parcellation within the DMN is generally consistent with the two-component model in humans, with one centred around the medial dlPFC and the other centred around PCC, but surprisingly the TPJ region in macaques is more closely related to dlPFC than the posteromedial parts of the DMN despite being anatomically closer to the latter (Mantini et al., 2011). One potentially feasible explanation for this discrepancy is the different axon tract development in macaques: whereas the anterior-posterior connection within the human DMN mainly involves the cingulum and SFOF through the temporal lobe (Teipel et al., 2010), SFOF is not as well-developed in macaques and its function is instead likely performed by the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) through the temporal lobe to TPJ; on the other hand, the forceps major tract system that connects most of the posteromedial and posterolateral cortices appears less extensive than in humans (Feng et al., 2017). Apart from that, the well-developed frontal decussating tract through the genu (ibid.) and dense connections within the posteromedial cortex (Parvizi et al., 2006) support the high degree of internal integrity within the frontal-temporal and posteromedial DMN sub-networks of macaques (hypothesis (b)), which agree with the overall DMN sub-division in humans.
Though there have been few studies investigating the marmoset DMN homologue, initial fMRI-based results show fairly similar results to macaques, with the DMN relatively isolated from other brain networks (hypothesis (a)), consisting of one main component around PCC, precuneus, and medial posterior parietal cortex, and another main component involving MTC, dmPFC, and medial parts of dlPFC (A46D, also slightly lateral to the human homologue; hypothesis (b)). However, one marked disparity with macaques and humans is the low FC between vmPFC and other putative DMN component regions, with A45 in lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) more involved in the frontotemporal DMN-homologue sub-network instead (Belcher et al., 2013; Hori et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2019). These results can be further confirmed by a battery of retrograde-tracing studies which confirmed extensive reciprocal connections amongst PCC, dmPFC, TPJ, precuneus, and showed little connection with vmPFC (Buckner & Margulies, 2019; Majka et al., 2020; Majka et al., 2016). Subcortical DMN connectivity as revealed by resting-state fMRI showed congruent FC with the midline thalamic nuclei but also strong connectivity with the superior colliculus which is not found in humans or macaques (Hori et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2019).
In conclusion, though the existence of a DMN homologue and its subdivision into anterior and posterior sub-networks is common to both model NHPs and humans, the following developments are likely apomorphic to the common ancestors of humans and chimpanzees: (a) medial shift of the frontal DMN hub; (b) enhanced frontoparietal connectivity, likely by means of increased development of the dorsal fronto-occipital pathway. The subcortical connectivity of DMN also diverged greatly among primate species which might limit clinical translation of relevant studies.
Default-like network in rodents
Despite the difficulty in measuring task-related brain activity in rodents, the anatomical conservation of the DMN component regions suggests the possibility of a functional homologue of DMN in rodents, which has been extensively investigated in resting-state fMRI studies. Results generally agree upon a highly conserved network centred around the retrosplenial cortex (RSP), but its components may vary greatly with respect to the analysis methods, animal wakefulness, and the particular anaesthetic used. Upadhyay and colleagues (2011) also identified the habituation of the awake animal to the scanner environment, a key factor affecting the resting-state connectivity, with many default-like behaviours only emerging after habituation, including the ACC-RSP, ACC-thalamic, ACC-temporo-association (TEA, the homologue of human TPJ) connections. Despite these varying factors, the existence of a closely connected network extending through the cingulate and bilaterally from RSP is generally agreed (see Figure 1), which is consistent with findings from primates. However, unlike in primate species, the DMN identified in many such studies also include wide ranges of primary sensory and motor areas, mostly in the visual cortex but also the S1/M1 and auditory areas which are known to be activated by sensory stimulations (Bigelow et al., 2022; C. Lee et al., 2022; C. C. Y. Lee et al., 2020; J. Y. Li et al., 2021). One explanation for this anomaly is the enhancement of temporal coherence of blood-oxygen-level-dependent signals by adjacency or shared vasculature. Indeed, the most robust network hub as shown in these studies (RSP, ACC) show some reciprocal neuron projections and dense projections within the posteromedial cortex as demonstrated by retrograde tracing, whereas projection density between these areas and the primary areas are very low (Oh et al., 2014). However, several other connections implicated in the primate DMN, including TPJ-PCC (rodent TEA-RSP), and PFC-MTC, are also not found in retrograde tracing (ibid.). Hence, the functional coupling between rodent RSP and wide ranges of the cortex (including primary areas) is likely indirectly mediated through subcortical structures, most likely the thalamus, which relay neural signals to diffuse cortical areas. High connectivity between the DMN and midline thalamus, hippocampus, and superior colliculus (Hsu et al., 2016) might support this relay theory. The reduction in cortico-cortical connectivity might constitute a major cross-species gap between the rodent and primate DMN. Despite this gap, the existence of a DMN homologue along the midline cortical areas (hypothesis (a)) is highly likely given the highly conserved connectivity pattern.
Figure 1
Flattened Representation of the Rodent DMN From Different Study Methods

Note. Red: global ICA, anaesthetised (Hutchison et al., 2010); Yellow: RSP-seeded, anaesthetised (Lu et al., 2012); Blue: graph-theory based modularity analysis, anaesthetised (Hsu et al., 2016); Green: seed-based, anaesthetised (Stafford et al., 2014); Grey: ACC-seeded, awake (Upadhyay et al., 2011); Pink: RSP-seeded, anaesthetised (N. Zhang et al., 2010); Cyan: graph-theory based modularity analysis, anaesthetised (Liska et al., 2015). Template adapted from (Paxinos & Franklin, 2004).
The quest into the internal organisation of the rodent DMN homologue further filled in the cross-species gap by discovering its anterior/posterior subdivision, highly similar to that of primate species (hypothesis (b)). Whilst posteromedial structures and secondary visual (cf. primate precuneus) areas and frontal medial cortices (ACC and prelimbic/infralimbic, cf. primate vmPFC; Vogt & Paxinos, 2014) are consensually assigned to distinct clusters, there are disputes regarding the cluster assignment of TEA (Cui et al., 2018; Hsu et al., 2016; Lu et al., 2012). Whilst both clusters are supported by dense regional projections, TEA does not show significant projection to either cluster anatomically (Oh et al., 2014), so large cross-study variation may result from indirect connections between the hubs and TEA. One of the stronger indirect pathways to TEA projects from ACC to lOFC (ibid.) and from lOFC to TEA (Hoover & Vertes, 2011). Since co-activation of ACC and TEA relayed via lOFC may lead to the development of a direct frontotemporal projection, this pathway in rodents is likely homologous to the frontotemporal DMN subnetwork in marmosets, which also includes lOFC and MTC.
In conclusion, there is likely a DMN homologue in rodents with similar activity, connectivity, and anterior-posterior subdivision to primates, but with reduced cortico-cortical connectivity. The anatomical evolution of DMN likely begins with the development of direct frontotemporal projections (as illustrated by marmosets) in primate species, followed by the development of fronto-parieto-occipital pathways (SFOF and cingulum), which likely happened when Old World monkeys diverged. The latter pathway in the process then gains in connection strength, resulting in the highly continuous DMN in humans today. The reduced need for relays via subcortical nuclei is likely a factor in the remodelling of subcortical connections of DMN. Cortical expansion and the accompanying increase in local connections is also a marked development in DMN evolution.
FUNCTIONAL CORRELATES OF DMN
Whilst there is a wealth of literature exploring the function of DMN in humans (see Andrews-Hanna, 2012; W. Q. Li et al., 2014; Stawarczyk et al., 2021; R. Zhang & Volkow, 2019), a major problem for pre-clinical translation is the difficulty to replicate complex DMN-related human behaviour in animals, including moral judgement (Marin-Morales et al., 2022), semantic processing (G. Zhang et al., 2022), and autobiographical memory (Philippi et al., 2015). Nevertheless, initial progress has been made into preclinical translational study of numerous known DMN-related disorders (ADHD, ASD, and Alzheimer’s), with various innovative tasks devised to investigate NHP DMN functions in attention, social interaction, and ageing.
Attention
Three studies so far have suggested that the NHP DMN is responsible for the shift of attention during cognitive tasks. Premereur and colleagues (2018) trained rhesus macaques to perform a visually guided eye movement task and a visually guided arm movement task, where the target cues differ only in colour (monkey focuses on the same red fixation cross, a green target directs eye movement, and a blue target instructs arm movement). Compared to “stay” situations, where monkeys do two tasks of the same kind successively, multiple areas of the frontotemporal sub-network of DMN are more activated when monkeys switch in either direction between eye movement and arm movement tasks, including medial dlPFC (including frontal eye field), lOFC, ACC, and MTC. Importantly, increased event-related activation of the same areas is observed for macaques returning from either task to fixation, compared to continuous fixation, indicating that these effects are likely task-nonspecific. Increased frontotemporal DMN activity was also implicated in covert attention shifts in Arsenault and colleagues’ study (2018), who derived a sensorimotor task where macaques are exposed to a pair of relevant/irrelevant stimuli on the left/right sides of the screen when fixated to the centre of the display. When the relevant (but not irrelevant) stimulus dims, the macaque can interrupt a light beam to get a reward. The random timing of dimming events requires macaques to maintain covert attention (attention without observable fixation) on the relevant target to maximise reward. Event-related fMRI recordings show an increased activation in the frontotemporal DMN subnetwork (ACC, lOFC, medial dlPFC, ACC, TPJ) and PPC in shifts of covert attention in either direction between L/R compared to “stays” of covert attention. These results are highly consistent with human cognitive control studies which also show task-switching-associated activations in human mPFC, medial dlPFC, ACC, MTC, TPJ, and PPC (Dibbets et al., 2010; Lemire-Rodger et al., 2019). In contrast, Hayden and colleagues (2010) observed decreased PCC activity in preparation for task switches, with the type of task (visual attention or working memory) indicated by an advance cue. However, it should be noted that the anticipated (proactive) task switching in this study differs from the reactive task switching in the above studies, which might account for the difference.
Importantly, deficits in task switching is a marked symptom of ADHD (Cepeda et al., 2000; King et al., 2007; Luna-Rodriguez et al., 2018), and aberrant activation patterns are observed in ADHD patients in both anticipated (Cubillo et al., 2010) and reactive (Dibbets et al., 2010) task-switching paradigms, including aberrant deactivation in OFC, striatum and thalamus, activation of precuneus, and reduced local homogeneity in cingulate activity. As these regions largely overlap with the DMN, especially the cingulate region, which agree with the frontoparietal transition of the NHP frontotemporal DMN subnetwork, the high degree of functional conservation of the DMN in macaque attention functions would open up wide possibilities for preclinical studies.
Social interactions
Many DMN component regions have been found to be associated with social functions in NHPs, especially in monitoring the actions of others. One innovative study design by Ninomiya and colleagues (2020) pairs two macaques together who switch between actor and observer roles; each macaque needs to plan its own action of button-pressing based on whether the partner’s previous action results in a reward (both receive a reward for correct choices). By measuring single-neuron activities at different loci of the macaque brain during this task, dense populations of neurons showing selective response to partner action and mirror neurons (responding to both self and partner action) were identified in the mPFC (ibid.) and TPJ (Ninomiya et al., 2021), which, importantly, activate preferentially in interactions with real macaques instead of pre-recorded macaque videos and non-living control objects, highlighting potential complex social function underlying this activation pattern. Whilst the coupling between these areas in the role-switching task is not studied, the coincidence between these areas and the proposed frontotemporal DMN subnetwork in macaques indicates a significant role of the DMN in social cognition.
Particularly noteworthy is the close association of the mirror neurons (i.e., neurons activated by observing conspecific behaviour) in human social cognition, which consists of mPFC and ventral parietal cortex (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), with dysfunctions in mirror neurons closely associated with social impairments in autism spectrum disorders (Enticott et al., 2012; Oberman et al., 2005). Indeed, inhibiting the ventral premotor (containing upstream mirror neurons)-mPFC pathway in macaques greatly impaired the macaques’ performance in the role-switch task, especially when the partner made an error (Ninomiya et al., 2020). One plausible explanation is that the DMN compares mirror inputs and existing social memory in tracking the motion of conspecifics (so inhibiting mirror input will also impair social functions), thus calibrating social expectation. This hypothesis seems to gain support from the observation that macaque MTC/TPJ selectively activate in response to unexpected social events (Roumazeilles et al., 2021). Hence, although the distribution of mirror neurons is likely different between macaques (frontotemporal DMN subnetwork) and humans (frontoparietal DMN subnetwork), the overall conserved role of DMN in social functions across primates still makes NHPs a good model for ASD-related studies.
Other social correlates reported for the macaque DMN component regions include face recognition of conspecifics and predators by mPFC (Dinh et al., 2018), forming social preferences by ACC (Basile et al., 2020), and dealing with social status-related stress by mPFC (Howell et al., 2014). Whilst many social behaviours are difficult to translate to NHPs, carefully designed behavioural paradigms are revealing more social functions of the NHP DMN, thus specifying the boundary of cross-species translatability in social cognition.
Ageing
Cognitive decline as a result of senescence has been studied both structurally and functionally amongst NHPs, with the posteromedial cortex identified as a hotspot in both functional and structural development in ageing, consistent with the close link between the parietal lobe and AD in humans (for review, see Bruner & Jacobs, 2013). One recent study identified decreased performance in object-recognition working memory and associative learning, but not short-term spatial memory, in aged macaques,[2] and correlated this decline with the degeneration of perineural networks around parvalbumin-expressing neurons in the PCC (Gray et al., 2022), a phenomenon also observed in mice and associated with aberrant firing in RSP of aged mice (Ueno et al., 2019). Importantly, this abnormality in PCC/RSP is shown to be only related to cognitive impairments instead of healthy ageing, as shown by the wide-ranging reduction of PCC/RSP FC in rats with memory impairments compared to age-comparable healthy rats (Ash et al., 2016), and in AD patients compared to healthy humans (Wu et al., 2011; H.-Y. Zhang et al., 2009). Though structural MRI studies found an increase in the volume of the PCC of macaques with age (Alexander et al., 2008; Koo et al., 2012), the loss of perineural networks provides a potential explanation for the functional decline despite volume increase: the firing rate of RSP pyramidal neurons greatly increases in the absence of perineural networks (Ueno et al., 2019), which might both increase local volume and promote the formation of a local circuitry, decreasing FC with other brain regions. However, in contrast to cognitively impaired rodents, AD patients show increased FC between PCC and the left somatosensory/motor areas (H.-Y. Zhang et al., 2009). This evolutionary gap in ageing/cognitive impairment-related behaviour is yet to be further investigated in NHPs.
Whilst there are few studies characterising the behavioural correlates of DMN in aged NHP, structural studies have provided initial evidence for the evolutionary continuity between the ageing process of human and macaque DMN: the conservation of volume in ageing in several regions of the frontoparietal DMN, including ACC, PCC (Alexander et al., 2008; Koo et al., 2012), and PFC (O’Donnell et al., 1999) compared to primary areas are highly consistent with the observation with aged humans (60 years and above; Frangou et al., 2022). One evolutionary gap identified in structural ageing studies is the dramatic decrease in lOFC volume in macaques which is not observed in humans (Alexander et al., 2008). It should however be noticed that lOFC does not constitute the DMN in humans. Hence, the structural similarity in DMN ontogeny between humans and NHP calls for more functional characterisation in aged NHPs.
CONCLUSION
The brain’s default mode network is perhaps the least functionally interpretable network of the brain, being most distant from the primary sensor/motor areas and showing general deactivation in tasks. As a result, despite wide-ranging DMN-related psychiatric disorders, pre-clinical studies of the DMN are still limited. Deciphering the evolutionary developments of the DMN would provide valuable insights into the translation of pre-clinical studies but also inspire more causal approaches to higher cognitive processes. This article examines the functional and anatomical evolution of the DMN across common clinical model organisms and humans, thus identifying the existence of a DMN homologue across all these species. Milestones in the anatomical evolution are likely the development of the frontotemporal network in primates and the subsequent frontotemporal-to-frontoparietal transition in the ancestors of chimpanzees and humans. Whilst the functional correlates of DMN in NHP are still poorly characterised due to difficulty of task design, initial studies reflect functional patterns generally coincident with this anatomical transition. Hence, more detailed characterisation of animal DMN-related behaviour would be an exciting frontier in research, with the objective of establishing the DMN as a potent clinical target.
NOTES
[1] Seed regions are regions whose resting state time series of blood-O2-level-dependent signals are correlated with all other ROIs or all voxels of the brain.
[2] Aged here refers to more than 22-year-old macaques, equivalent to 66-year-old humans (Tigges et al., 1988).
INTERDISCIPLINARY COMMENTARY
PSYCHOLOGY
Is the default mode network the locus of imagination and morality, and what adaptive functions do these psychological faculties serve that distinguish humans from other primates?
Eve Selwood-Metcalfe
Corpus Christ College, University of Cambridge
The default mode network (DMN) is an important neural network that has been implicated in a range of activities within human, nonhuman primate, cat, and rodent brains. It has consequently been linked to various neuropsychological disorders in humans, such as autism spectrum disorders, ADHD, and dementia. However, it has more recently been suggested that the DMN may be involved in more abstract psychological functions such as imagination, empathy and morality, qualities that have previously been difficult to investigate empirically and which seem to be largely unique to humans. This commentary will therefore aim to examine the potential role of the DMN in imagination and morality, considering their adaptive functions within humans in allowing both internal reflection and aiding social functions which have been crucial in characterising Homo sapiens.
Introduction
When the brain is not engaged in perception of the external world and subsequent directed action, it is not resting; it ‘defaults’ to activity in a neural network known as the default mode network (DMN; Carroll, 2020). The DMN consists of a range of brain regions that span from cortical areas—including the medial prefrontal cortex, the posterior cingulate cortex, the lateral parietal and temporal association cortices, and the medial temporal lobes (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Buckner & DiNicola, 2019; Molnar-Szakacs & Uddin, 2013)—to subcortical regions such as the amygdala and hippocampus. The numerous neural regions of the DMN are associated with a range of different activities and functions, from episodic memory to morality and theory of mind among many more. In this way, the DMN can allow the mind to flexibly switch between attending to the external world and focusing on the internal world of imagination and morality as unique psychological facilities of the modern human.
This review will firstly examine how the psychological function of imagination may be to some extent centred in the default mode network and associated areas, evaluating the functional evolutionand adaptive significance of this. It will then consider another proposed role of the DMN regarding morality and empathy, and how DMN abnormalities such as those associated with psychopathy and dementia can further elucidate the morality function of the DMN.
Imagination and the default mode network
Within the past decade, imagination has increasingly become the subject of empirical study in regards to the evolution of human cognition and cognitive neuroscience, allowing us to pinpoint such a uniquely human function to distinct regions of the brain as an identifiable set of cognitive components that serve adaptive functions (Carroll, 2020). In this way, imagination proves itself as an important factor in making humans a distinct, dominant species. Imagination can be defined as “the capacity to mentally transcend time, place and/or circumstance to think about what might have been, plan and anticipate the future, create fictional worlds, and consider remote and close alternatives to actual experiences” (Taylor, 2013, p. 791), with the brain inwardly focused on mental representations which are dissociated from the external environment. Such a definition seems to be consistent with the DMN, which has aptly been described as the “imagination network” (Kaufman & Gregoire, 2015, p. xxvii), the neurological locus of imagination. Buckner and colleagues (2008) describe the DMN as critically allowing the brain to simulate alternative perspectives to the present, supporting both self-directed functions such as daydreaming, as well as a more social function regarding imagining the inner life of others (i.e., theory of mind; Pace-Schlott, 2013).
Imagination seems to be a defining feature of the human experience, and though there is not conclusive evidence that non-human primates (NHP) are incapable of some degree of imagination, it seems reasonable to assume human imagination is somewhat unparalleled in other animals (Suddendorf, 2013). Humans are able to reflect on their own experiences, creating a life narrative, and imagine the inner lives of others (McAdams, 2015; McLean, 2016), as well as qualifying their imaginative conceptions of the world with moral values and even supernatural forces. When looking at the evolution of brain regions which may have allowed this unique function to arise in humans, there is evidence that as modern Homo sapiens evolved, parietal bulging occurred (Neubauer et al., 2018), increasing the size of inner parietal regions. One such area is the praecuneus, a central node of the DMN which is implicated in mental imagery concerning the self, representing other peoples’ inner minds, and higher creativity (Benedek & Jauk, 2018). Therefore, as the modern brain (and consequently the DMN) was evolving, Homo sapiens were becoming behaviourally modern and thus more imaginative, and this modern human faculty of imagination allowed cognitive and behavioural flexibility unique to humans (Margulies et al., 2016; Schacter, 2018; Schacter et al., 2018). This flexibility is closely linked to the aforementioned creativity, and both are powerful prerequisites for the modernisation of human behaviour.
There are three main processes of imagination that seem to be supported by the DMN: simulation, mental time travel, and perspective taking (or theory of mind), which then provide a basis for more complex imaginative functions such as dreaming, mind-wandering, auto-biographical narratives, counterfactual thinking, fantasising, moral reflection, comprehending narratives, and formulating intentional fictional constructs. Simulation is broadly understood as “mental representation”, essential for all imaginative experience (Buckner et al., 2008; Molnar-Szakacs & Uddin, 2013; Oatley, 2016; Roese & Epstude, 2017; Schacter, 2018; Tamir et al., 2015). Mental time travel is conscious perception of “personal identity as a continuous stream of experience over time” (Carroll, 2020, p. 38), allowing humans to make use of past and present memories to construct imaginative conceptions of the future. This flexible future planning has been proposed as an adaptive force which drives the expansion of human imagination, allowing humans to plan and prepare effectively for future contingencies (Andrews-Hanna, 2012; Buckner, 2012; Schacter, 2018). Finally, perspective taking allows navigation of the human social environment through an imaginative awareness of other people as individual agents with their own stream of consciousness, and their own desires, fears, beliefs, and values. This therefore allows humans to anticipate the thoughts and actions of others, predict their motives, and respond accordingly. The adaptive value of these three processes of imagination are supported by the fact that there are such substantial costs to these functions, yet they still exist within human behaviour. The DMN requires huge amounts of metabolic energy to function, and there are also risks associated with guiding behaviour based on imaginative conceptions of reality, as they can often prove to be wrong. Therefore, imagination seems to be essentially valuable as a human faculty with great adaptive functional power, otherwise it would have been selected against throughout the process of human evolution.
Therefore, imagination as produced in the DMN has adaptive functions which have driven the evolution of the human species and its unique behavioural faculties. Humans are able to utilise their capacity for dreaming, mind-wandering, autobiographical narratives, counterfactual thinking, and so on, to produce a conception of the self, understanding the inner lives of others to satisfy social goals (both aiding cooperative behaviour within communities, as well as competitive manipulation between individuals), creating links between imaginative ideas, and appreciating multiple possible courses of action depending on conceptions of the future. These functions allow the creation of an imaginative inner world that can guide behaviour in the external world.
Morality and the default mode network
As with imagination, moral decision making is also an internally-directed function that has been associated with the DMN. We use internal representations of our personal norms and values to formulate beliefs of what is right and wrong, and this contributes to our actions within the external world. Reniers and colleagues (2012) used fMRI to compare moral and non-moral decision-making using scenarios that required deliberate contemplation, examining the relationship between blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal and moral judgement competence, psychopathy, and empathy. They found that there were greater levels of activity in the DMN during moral decision-making, compared to non-moral decision making. High scores on primary psychopathy also correlated with a decreased difference between moral versus non-moral decision-making BOLD activation, suggesting that psychopathy is associated with lower activation of the DMN for moral decisions—i.e., moral decisions are viewed similarly to non-moral decisions. The study concluded that moral decision making demands a greater degree of internally directed processing (through the functioning of the DMN), including processes such as self-referential mental processing and the representation of intentions and feelings. Furthermore, Han (2017) meta-analysed studies investigating the neural mechanism of morality, by similarly comparing neural activity in moral task conditions to that in non-moral task conditions. Focusing on the neural correlates of both moral sensitivity and judgement (two functional components in the Neo-Kohlbergian model of moral functioning), the study showed that brain regions associated with the DMN were significantly more active for morality-related task conditions, compared to non-morality task conditions. These brain regions were also shown to be activated in both moral sensitivity and judgement task conditions, suggesting that the DMN is associated with multiple dimensions of morality. In conclusion, the study suggests that moral functioning is associated with self-related psychological processes occurring in the DMN.
We can understand the association between the DMN and morality by examining the consequences for moral understanding as a result of a breakdown of the DMN, as shown in some cases of psychopathy and dementia.
Pujol and colleagues (2012) used fMRI data to investigate alterations in the neural network supporting moral judgement (assumed to be the DMN) in criminal psychopaths, hypothesising that these alterations are not just confined to inadequate use of networks during moral judgement, but that a wider network breakdown would exist within these psychopaths, with dysfunctional alterations beyond moral dilemma situations. To do this, they measured (1) brain response to moral dilemmas; (2) task-induced deactivation of the network during a cognitive task; and (3) the strength of functional connectivity within the network. Results showed that the network dominating moral judgements (the DMN) was underactive in psychopathic subjects during moral dilemma situations. However, the study also demonstrated that there is a baseline network alteration beyond moral contexts in psychopaths, with a functional disconnection between emotional and cognitive elements that together form moral judgement. Therefore, psychopathy and the associated reduced moral judgement may, at least partly, be a result of a breakdown of the DMN, further elucidating the network’s significance in the psychological capacity of morality.
There is also a breakdown of the DMN and morality in dementia, as extrapolated by Chiong and colleagues (2013), who looked at decreased DMN functional activation during personal moral judgements in frontotemporal dementia patients. Previous research has shown that behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) is characterised by decreased resting-state functional connectivity within the DMN (Zhou et al., 2010). However, the link between network changes and changes in patient’s behaviour is complex. For example, decision-making in personal moral dilemmas typically activates DMN nodes, yet choice behaviour in such dilemmas is unusually utilitarian in bvFTD (Mendez & Shapira, 2009), with patients being more likely to endorse inflicting harm on an innocent person to save a greater number of people, compared to healthy controls. Using fMRI data, the study showed that healthy control participants exhibited greater activation in nodes of the DMN (praecuneus, medial prefrontal cortex, and angular gyri) in moral personal dilemmas, compared to in nonmoral dilemmas or moral-impersonal dilemmas. bvFTD patients were, however, more likely to endorse utilitarian infringements of personal rights and had decreased praecuneus activation in moral-personal dilemmas, compared to healthy controls. As with psychopathy, the dysfunction of the DMN associated with dementia can be used to demonstrate the role of the DMN in personal moral reasoning.
Conclusion
Despite the difficulty researching abstract psychological functions such as imagination and morality, it does seem that at least to some degree both faculties are related to the functioning of the DMN, allowing internal reflective function such as that involved in imagination, relating the external world to internal life, linking past, present and future, and allowing complex creative thought that is largely unique to humans. The DMN also appears important in a wider social context, further demonstrating its functional adaptiveness, as it has a role in understanding the internal minds of others and effectively functioning in a social context.
However, much more work needs to be done to firstly create more empirical and reliable definitions of concepts such as imagination and morality, as understandings of this vary across literature. It is also important to understand that such complex human psychology cannot be limited to one region of the brain, or even one network, and likely involves a range of interconnected webs of activity across the brain. Overall, the DMN does seem to be a significant brain network for a range of human functions.
ARTICLE REFERENCES
Alexander, G. E., Chen, K., Aschenbrenner, M., Merkley, T. L., Santerre-Lemmon, L. E., Shamy, J. L., Skaggs, W. E., Buonocore, M. H., Rapp, P. R., & Barnes, C. A. (2008). Age-related regional network of magnetic resonance imaging gray matter in the rhesus macaque. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 2710–2718. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1852-07.2008
Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18(3), 251–270. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858411403316
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Saxe, R., & Yarkoni, T. (2014). Contributions of episodic retrieval and mentalizing to autobiographical thought: Evidence from functional neuroimaging, resting-state connectivity, and fMRI meta-analyses. NeuroImage, 91, 324–335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.032
Arsenault, J. T., Caspari, N., Vandenberghe, R., & Vanduffel, W. (2018). Attention shifts recruit the monkey default mode network. The Journal of Neuroscience, 38(5), 1202–1217. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1111-17.2017
Ash, J. A., Lu, H., Taxier, L. R., Long, J. M., Yang, Y., Stein, E. A., & Rapp, P. R. (2016). Functional connectivity with the retrosplenial cortex predicts cognitive aging in rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(43), 12286–12291. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1525309113
Barks, S. K., Parr, L. A., & Rilling, J. K. (2015). The default mode network in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) is similar to that of humans. Cerebral Cortex, 25(2), 538–544. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht253
Basile, B. M., Schafroth, J. L., Karaskiewicz, C. L., Chang, S. W. C., & Murray, E. A. (2020). The anterior cingulate cortex is necessary for forming prosocial preferences from vicarious reinforcement in monkeys. PLOS Biology, 18(6), Article e3000677. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000677
Basser, P. J. (1995). Inferring microstructural features and the physiological state of tissues from diffusion-weighted images. NMR in Biomedicine, 8(7), 333–344. https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.1940080707
Belcher, A. M., Yen, C. C., Stepp, H., Gu, H., Lu, H., Yang, Y., Silva, A. C., & Stein, E. A. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in the awake, truly resting marmoset monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(42), 16796–16804. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3146-13.2013
Bigelow, J., Morrill, R. J., Olsen, T., & Hasenstaub, A. R. (2022). Visual modulation of firing and spectrotemporal receptive fields in mouse auditory cortex. Current Research in Neurobiology, 3, 100040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crneur.2022.100040
Biswal, B., Zerrin Yetkin, F., Haughton, V. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 34(4), 537-541. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910340409
Bruner, E., & Jacobs, H. I. L. (2013). Alzheimer’s disease: The downside of a highly evolved parietal lobe? Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 35(2), 227–240. https://doi.org/10.3233/jad-122299
Buckner, R. L., & Margulies, D. S. (2019). Macroscale cortical organization and a default-like apex transmodal network in the marmoset monkey. Nature Communications, 10(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09812-8
Cepeda, N. J., Cepeda, M. L., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). Task switching and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(3), 213–226. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005143419092
Chiong, W., Wilson, S. M., D’Esposito, M., Kayser, A. S., Grossman, S. N., Poorzand, P., Seeley, W. W., Miller, B. L., & Rankin, K. P. (2013). The salience network causally influences default mode network activity during moral reasoning. Brain, 136(6), 1929–1941. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt066
Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Ecker, C., Giampietro, V., Taylor, E., & Rubia, K. (2010). Reduced activation and inter-regional functional connectivity of fronto-striatal networks in adults with childhood Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and persisting symptoms during tasks of motor inhibition and cognitive switching. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44(10), 629–639. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.11.016
Cui, Y., Yu, S., Zhang, T., Zhang, Y., Xia, Y., Yao, D., & Guo, D. (2018). Altered activity and information flow in the default mode network of pilocarpine-induced epilepsy rats. Brain Research, 1696, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2018.05.012
Desgranges, B., Baron, J.-C., & Eustache, F. (1998). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic memory: The role of the frontal lobes, the hippocampal formation, and other areas. NeuroImage, 8(2), 198–213. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0359
Dibbets, P., Evers, E. A. T., Hurks, P. P. M., Bakker, K., & Jolles, J. (2010). Differential brain activation patterns in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) associated with task switching. Neuropsychology, 24(4), 413–423. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018997
Dinh, H. T., Nishimaru, H., Matsumoto, J., Takamura, Y., Le, Q. V., Hori, E., Maior, R. S., Tomaz, C., Tran, A. H., Ono, T., & Nishijo, H. (2018). Superior neuronal detection of snakes and conspecific faces in the macaque medial prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 28(6), 2131–2145. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx118
Enticott, P. G., Kennedy, H. A., Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Taffe, J. R., Daskalakis, Z. J., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2012). Mirror neuron activity associated with social impairments but not age in autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 71(5), 427–433.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.001
Feng, L., Jeon, T., Yu, Q., Ouyang, M., Peng, Q., Mishra, V., Pletikos, M., Sestan, N., Miller, M. I., Mori, S., Hsiao, S., Liu, S., & Huang, H. (2017). Population-averaged macaque brain atlas with high-resolution ex vivo DTI integrated into in vivo space. Brain Structure and Function, 222(9), 4131–4147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-017-1463-6
Ferrier, J., Tiran, E., Deffieux, T., Tanter, M., & Lenkei, Z. (2020). Functional imaging evidence for task-induced deactivation and disconnection of a major default mode network hub in the mouse brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(26), 15270–15280. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1920475117
Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Amlien, I., Tamnes, C. K., Grydeland, H., Engvig, A., Espeseth, T., Reinvang, I., Lundervold, A. J., Lundervold, A., & Walhovd, K. B. (2015). High-expanding cortical regions in human development and evolution are related to higher intellectual abilities. Cerebral Cortex, 25(1), 26–34. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht201
Frangou, S., Modabbernia, A., Williams, S. C. R., Papachristou, E., Doucet, G. E., Agartz, I., Aghajani, M., Akudjedu, T. N., Albajes‐Eizagirre, A., Alnæs, D., Alpert, K. I., Andersson, M., Andreasen, N. C., Andreassen, O. A., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Bargallo, N., Baumeister, S., Baur‐Streubel, R., . . . Dima, D. (2022). Cortical thickness across the lifespan: Data from 17,075 healthy individuals aged 3–90 years. Human Brain Mapping, 43(1), 431–451. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25364
Gamberini, M., Passarelli, L., Impieri, D., Worthy, K. H., Burman, K. J., Fattori, P., Galletti, C., Rosa, M. G. P., & Bakola, S. (2020). Thalamic afferents emphasize the different functions of macaque precuneate areas. Brain Structure and Function, 225(2), 853–870. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02045-2
Gray, D. T., Khattab, S., Meltzer, J., McDermott, K., Schwyhart, R., Sinakevitch, I., Härtig, W., & Barnes, C. A. (2022). Retrosplenial cortex microglia and perineuronal net densities are associated with memory impairment in aged rhesus macaques. Cerebral Cortex, Article bhac366. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac366
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(1), 253–258. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0135058100
Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(13), 4637–4642. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0308627101
Hayden, B. Y., Smith, D. V., & Platt, M. L. (2010). Cognitive control signals in posterior cingulate cortex [Original Research]. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00223
Hayden, B. Y., Smith, D. V., & Platt, M. L. (2009). Electrophysiological correlates of default-mode processing in macaque posterior cingulate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(14), 5948–5953. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812035106
Hill, J., Inder, T., Neil, J., Dierker, D., Harwell, J., & Van Essen, D. (2010). Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(29), 13135–13140. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001229107
Hoover, W. B., & Vertes, R. P. (2011). Projections of the medial orbital and ventral orbital cortex in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 519(18), 3766–3801. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22733
Hori, Y., Cléry, J. C., Schaeffer, D. J., Menon, R. S., & Everling, S. (2022). Functional organization of frontoparietal cortex in the marmoset investigated with awake resting-state fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 32(9), 1965–1977. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab328
Hori, Y., Schaeffer, D. J., Yoshida, A., Cléry, J. C., Hayrynen, L. K., Gati, J. S., Menon, R. S., & Everling, S. (2020). Cortico-subcortical functional connectivity profiles of resting-state networks in marmosets and humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 40(48), 9236–9249. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1984-20.2020
Howell, B. R., Godfrey, J., Gutman, D. A., Michopoulos, V., Zhang, X., Nair, G., Hu, X., Wilson, M. E., & Sanchez, M. M. (2014). Social subordination stress and serotonin transporter polymorphisms: Associations with brain white matter tract integrity and behavior in juvenile female macaques. Cerebral Cortex, 24(12), 3334–3349. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht187
Hsu, L.-M., Liang, X., Gu, H., Brynildsen, J. K., Stark, J. A., Ash, J. A., Lin, C.-P., Lu, H., Rapp, P. R., Stein, E. A., & Yang, Y. (2016). Constituents and functional implications of the rat default mode network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(31), E4541–E4547. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601485113
Hutchison, R. M., Leung, L. S., Mirsattari, S. M., Gati, J. S., Menon, R. S., & Everling, S. (2011). Resting-state networks in the macaque at 7T. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1546–1555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.063
Hutchison, R. M., Mirsattari, S. M., Jones, C. K., Gati, J. S., & Leung, L. S. (2010). Functional networks in the anesthetized rat brain revealed by independent component analysis of resting-state fMRI. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(6), 3398–3406. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00141.2010
King, J. A., Colla, M., Brass, M., Heuser, I., & Von Cramon, D. (2007). Inefficient cognitive control in adult ADHD: evidence from trial-by-trial Stroop test and cued task switching performance. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(1), 42. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-42
Kojima, T., Onoe, H., Hikosaka, K., Tsutsui, K. I., Tsukada, H., & Watanabe, M. (2009). Default mode of brain activity demonstrated by positron emission tomography imaging in awake monkeys: Higher rest-related than working memory-related activity in medial cortical areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(46), 14463–14471. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1786-09.2009
Koo, B.-B., Schettler, S. P., Murray, D. E., Lee, J.-M., Killiany, R. J., Rosene, D. L., Kim, D.-S., & Ronen, I. (2012). Age-related effects on cortical thickness patterns of the Rhesus monkey brain. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(1), 200.e223–200.e231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.010
Lee, C., Kim, Y., & Kaang, B.-K. (2022). The primary motor cortex: The hub of motor learning in rodents. Neuroscience, 485, 163–170./https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.01.009
Lee, C. C. Y., Kheradpezhouh, E., Diamond, M. E., & Arabzadeh, E. (2020). State-dependent changes in perception and coding in the mouse somatosensory cortex. Cell Reports, 32(13), 108197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108197
Lemire-Rodger, S., Lam, J., Viviano, J. D., Stevens, W. D., Spreng, R. N., & Turner, G. R. (2019). Inhibit, switch, and update: A within-subject fMRI investigation of executive control. Neuropsychologia, 132, Article 107134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107134
Li, J., Curley, W. H., Guerin, B., Dougherty, D. D., Dalca, A. V., Fischl, B., Horn, A., & Edlow, B. L. (2021). Mapping the subcortical connectivity of the human default mode network. NeuroImage, 245, Article 118758. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118758
Li, J., Martin, S., Tricklebank, M. D., Schwarz, A. J., & Gilmour, G. (2015). Task-induced modulation of intrinsic functional connectivity networks in the behaving rat. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(2), 658–665. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3488-14.2015
Li, J. Y., Hass, C. A., Matthews, I., Kristl, A. C., & Glickfeld, L. L. (2021). Distinct recruitment of feedforward and recurrent pathways across higher-order areas of mouse visual cortex. Current Biology, 31(22), 5024–5036.e5025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.09.042
Li, W. Q., Mai, X. Q., & Liu, C. (2014). The default mode network and social understanding of others: what do brain connectivity studies tell us. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(74). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00074
Liska, A., Galbusera, A., Schwarz, A. J., & Gozzi, A. (2015). Functional connectivity hubs of the mouse brain. NeuroImage, 115, 281–291.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.033
Liu, C., Yen, C. C.-C., Szczupak, D., Ye, F. Q., Leopold, D. A., & Silva, A. C. (2019). Anatomical and functional investigation of the marmoset default mode network. Nature Communications, 10(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09813-7
Lu, H., Zou, Q., Gu, H., Raichle, M. E., Stein, E. A., & Yang, Y. (2012). Rat brains also have a default mode network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(10), 3979–3984. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200506109
Luna-Rodriguez, A., Wendt, M., Kerner Auch Koerner, J., Gawrilow, C., & Jacobsen, T. (2018). Selective impairment of attentional set shifting in adults with ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 14(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-018-0150-y
Majka, P., Bai, S., Bakola, S., Bednarek, S., Chan, J. M., Jermakow, N., Passarelli, L., Reser, D. H., Theodoni, P., Worthy, K. H., Wang, X.-J., Wójcik, D. K., Mitra, P. P., & Rosa, M. G. P. (2020). Open access resource for cellular-resolution analyses of corticocortical connectivity in the marmoset monkey. Nature Communications, 11(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-14858-0
Majka, P., Chaplin, T. A., Yu, H.-H., Tolpygo, A., Mitra, P. P., Wójcik, D. K., & Rosa, M. G. P. (2016). Towards a comprehensive atlas of cortical connections in a primate brain: Mapping tracer injection studies of the common marmoset into a reference digital template. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 524(11), 2161–2181. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24023
Mantini, D., Gerits, A., Nelissen, K., Durand, J. B., Joly, O., Simone, L., Sawamura, H., Wardak, C., Orban, G. A., Buckner, R. L., & Vanduffel, W. (2011). Default mode of brain function in monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(36), 12954–12962. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2318-11.2011
Margulies, D. S., Ghosh, S. S., Goulas, A., Falkiewicz, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Langs, G., Bezgin, G., Eickhoff, S. B., Castellanos, F. X., Petrides, M., Jefferies, E., & Smallwood, J. (2016). Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(44), 12574–12579. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608282113
Margulies, D. S., Vincent, J. L., Kelly, C., Lohmann, G., Uddin, L. Q., Biswal, B. B., Villringer, A., Castellanos, F. X., Milham, M. P., & Petrides, M. (2009). Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(47), 20069–20074. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905314106
Marin-Morales, A., Bueso-Izquierdo, N., Hidalgo-Ruzzante, N., Perez-Garcia, M., Catena-Martinez, A., & Verdejo-Roman, J. (2022). “Would you allow your wife to dress in a miniskirt to the party”? Batterers do not activate default mode network during moral decisions about intimate partner violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 37(3-4), NP1463–NP1488. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260520926494
Miller, D. R., Hayes, S. M., Hayes, J. P., Spielberg, J. M., Lafleche, G., & Verfaellie, M. (2017). Default mode network subsystems are differentially disrupted in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(4), 363–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.12.006
Ninomiya, T., Noritake, A., & Isoda, M. (2021). Live agent preference and social action monitoring in the macaque mid-superior temporal sulcus region. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(44), Article e2109653118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2109653118
Ninomiya, T., Noritake, A., Kobayashi, K., & Isoda, M. (2020). A causal role for frontal cortico-cortical coordination in social action monitoring. Nature Communications, 11(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19026-y
O’Donnell, K. A., Rapp, P. R., & Hof, P. R. (1999). Preservation of prefrontal cortical volume in behaviorally characterized aged macaque monkeys. Experimental Neurology, 160(1), 300–310. https://doi.org/10.1006/exnr.1999.7192
Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014
Oh, S. W., Harris, J. A., Ng, L., Winslow, B., Cain, N., Mihalas, S., Wang, Q., Lau, C., Kuan, L., Henry, A. M., Mortrud, M. T., Ouellette, B., Nguyen, T. N., Sorensen, S. A., Slaughterbeck, C. R., Wakeman, W., Li, Y., Feng, D., Ho, A., . . . Zeng, H. (2014). A mesoscale connectome of the mouse brain. Nature, 508(7495), 207–214. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13186
Padmanabhan, A., Lynch, C. J., Schaer, M., & Menon, V. (2017). The default mode network in autism. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(6), 476–486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.04.004
Parvizi, J., Van Hoesen, G. W., Buckwalter, J., & Damasio, A. (2006). Neural connections of the posteromedial cortex in the macaque. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(5), 1563–1568. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0507729103
Passow, S., Specht, K., Adamsen, T. C., Biermann, M., Brekke, N., Craven, A. R., Ersland, L., Grüner, R., Kleven‐Madsen, N., Kvernenes, O. H., Schwarzlmüller, T., Olesen, R. A., & Hugdahl, K. (2015). Default‐mode network functional connectivity is closely related to metabolic activity. Human Brain Mapping, 36(6), 2027–2038. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22753
Paxinos, G., & Franklin, K. B. J. (2004). The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates (Compact 2nd ed.). Elsevier Academic Press.
Paxinos, G., Petrides, M., Tokuno, H., Watson, C., & Rosa, M. (2012). The marmoset brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Academic Press. ISBN: 9780124158184
Philippi, C. L., Tranel, D., Duff, M., & Rudrauf, D. (2015). Damage to the default mode network disrupts autobiographical memory retrieval. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(3), 318–326. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu070
Premereur, E., Janssen, P., & Vanduffel, W. (2018). Functional MRI in macaque monkeys during task switching. The Journal of Neuroscience, 38(50), 10619–10630. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1539-18.2018
Raichle, M. E., Macleod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676–682. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676
Reveley, C., Gruslys, A., Ye, F. Q., Glen, D., Samaha, J., E. Russ, B., Saad, Z., K. Seth, A., Leopold, D. A., & Saleem, K. S. (2017). Three-dimensional digital template atlas of the macaque brain. Cerebral Cortex, 27(9), 4463–4477. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw248
Rilling, J. K., Barks, S. K., Parr, L. A., Preuss, T. M., Faber, T. L., Pagnoni, G., Bremner, J. D., & Votaw, J. R. (2007). A comparison of resting-state brain activity in humans and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17146–17151. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705132104
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27(1), 169–192. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
Roumazeilles, L., Schurz, M., Lojkiewiez, M., Verhagen, L., Schüffelgen, U., Marche, K., Mahmoodi, A., Emberton, A., Simpson, K., Joly, O., Khamassi, M., Rushworth, M. F. S., Mars, R. B., & Sallet, J. (2021). Social prediction modulates activity of macaque superior temporal cortex. Science Advances, 7(38), Article eabh2392. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abh2392
Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 648–663. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648
Smallwood, J., Bernhardt, B. C., Leech, R., Bzdok, D., Jefferies, E., & Margulies, D. S. (2021). The default mode network in cognition: A topographical perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 22(8), 503–513. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-021-00474-4
Stafford, J. M., Jarrett, B. R., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Mills, B. D., Cain, N., Mihalas, S., Lahvis, G. P., Lattal, K. M., Mitchell, S. H., David, S. V., Fryer, J. D., Nigg, J. T., & Fair, D. A. (2014). Large-scale topology and the default mode network in the mouse connectome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(52), 18745–18750. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1404346111
Stawarczyk, D., Bezdek, M. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2021). Event representations and predictive processing: The role of the midline default network core. Topics in Cognitive Science, 13(1), 164–186. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12450
Teipel, S. J., Bokde, A. L. W., Meindl, T., Amaro, E., Soldner, J., Reiser, M. F., Herpertz, S. C., Möller, H.-J., & Hampel, H. (2010). White matter microstructure underlying default mode network connectivity in the human brain. NeuroImage, 49(3), 2021–2032. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.067
Tigges, J., Gordon, T. P., McClure, H. M., Hall, E. C., & Peters, A. (1988). Survival rate and life span of rhesus monkeys at the Yerkes regional primate research center. American Journal of Primatology, 15(3), 263–273. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350150308
Ueno, H., Fujii, K., Takao, K., Suemitsu, S., Murakami, S., Kitamura, N., Wani, K., Matsumoto, Y., Okamoto, M., & Ishihara, T. (2019). Alteration of parvalbumin expression and perineuronal nets formation in the cerebral cortex of aged mice. Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience, 95, 31–42. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mcn.2018.12.008
Upadhyay, J., Baker, S. J., Chandran, P., Miller, L., Lee, Y., Marek, G. J., Sakoglu, U., Chin, C.-L., Luo, F., Fox, G. B., & Day, M. (2011). Default-mode-like network activation in awake rodents. PLOS One, 6(11), Article e27839. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027839
Van Den Heuvel, M. P., Mandl, R. C. W., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009). Functionally linked resting-state networks reflect the underlying structural connectivity architecture of the human brain. Human Brain Mapping, 30(10), 3127–3141. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20737
van Oort, E. S. B., van Cappellen van Walsum, A. M., & Norris, D. G. (2014). An investigation into the functional and structural connectivity of the default mode network. NeuroImage, 90, 381–389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.051
Vatansever, D., Menon, D. K., Manktelow, A. E., Sahakian, B. J., & Stamatakis, E. A. (2015). Default mode network connectivity during task execution. NeuroImage, 122, 96–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.053
Vogt, B. A., & Paxinos, G. (2014). Cytoarchitecture of mouse and rat cingulate cortex with human homologies. Brain Structure and Function, 219(1), 185–192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0493-3
Wen, T., Mitchell, D. J., & Duncan, J. (2020). The functional convergence and heterogeneity of social, episodic, and self-referential thought in the default mode network. Cerebral Cortex, 30(11), 5915–5929. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa166
Wu, X., Li, R., Fleisher, A. S., Reiman, E. M., Guan, X., Zhang, Y., Chen, K., & Yao, L. (2011). Altered default mode network connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease: A resting functional MRI and bayesian network study. Human Brain Mapping, 32(11), 1868–1881. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21153
Yan, C.-G., Chen, X., Li, L., Castellanos, F. X., Bai, T.-J., Bo, Q.-J., Cao, J., Chen, G.-M., Chen, N.-X., Chen, W., Cheng, C., Cheng, Y.-Q., Cui, X.-L., Duan, J., Fang, Y.-R., Gong, Q.-Y., Guo, W.-B., Hou, Z.-H., Hu, L., . . . Zang, Y.-F. (2019). Reduced default mode network functional connectivity in patients with recurrent major depressive disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(18), 9078–9083. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1900390116
Yeshurun, Y., Nguyen, M., & Hasson, U. (2021). The default mode network: Where the idiosyncratic self meets the shared social world. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 22(3), 181–192. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-00420-w
Yuan, Y., Pan, X., & Wang, R. (2021). Biophysical mechanism of the interaction between default mode network and working memory network. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 15(6), 1101–1124. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-021-09674-1
Zhang, G., Hung, J., & Lin, N. (2022). Coexistence of the social semantic effect and non-semantic effect in the default mode network. Brain Structure and Function. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-022-02476-z
Zhang, H.-Y., Wang, S.-J., Xing, J., Liu, B., Ma, Z.-L., Yang, M., Zhang, Z.-J., & Teng, G.-J. (2009). Detection of PCC functional connectivity characteristics in resting-state fMRI in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Behavioural Brain Research, 197(1), 103–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.012
Zhang, N., Rane, P., Huang, W., Liang, Z., Kennedy, D., Frazier, J. A., & King, J. (2010). Mapping resting-state brain networks in conscious animals. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 189(2), 186–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.001
Zhang, R., & Volkow, N. D. (2019). Brain default-mode network dysfunction in addiction. NeuroImage, 200, 313–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.036
Zhao, X.-H., Wang, P.-J., Li, C.-B., Hu, Z.-H., Xi, Q., Wu, W.-Y., & Tang, X.-W. (2007). Altered default mode network activity in patient with anxiety disorders: An fMRI study. European Journal of Radiology, 63(3), 373–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejrad.2007.02.006
COMMENTARY REFERENCES
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron, 65(4), 550–562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005
Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18(3), 251–270. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858411403316
Benedek, M., & Jauk, E. (2018). Spontaneous and controlled processes in creative cognition. In K. C. R. Fox, & K. Christoff (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and dreaming (pp. 285–298). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.22
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011
Buckner, R. L. (2012). The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. NeuroImage, 62(2), 1137–1145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.035
Buckner, R. L., & DiNicola, L. M. (2019). The brain’s default network: Updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(10), 593–608. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7
Carroll, J. (2020). Imagination, the brain’s default mode network, and imaginative verbal artifacts. In J. Carroll, M. Clasen, & E. Jonsson (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on imaginative culture (pp. 31–52). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-46190-4_2
Chiong, W., Wilson, S. M., D’Esposito, M., Kayser, A. S., Grossman, S. N., Poorzand, P., Seeley, W. W., Miller, B. L., & Rankin, K. P. (2013). The salience network causally influences default mode network activity during moral reasoning. Brain, 136(6), 1929–1941. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt066
Han, H. (2017). Neural correlates of moral sensitivity and moral judgment associated with brain circuitries of selfhood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Moral Education, 46(2), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2016.1262834
Kaufman, S. B., & Gregoire, C. (2015). Wired to create: Unraveling the mysteries of the creative mind (First Edition). Perigee Books. ISBN: 9780399174100
Margulies, D. S., Ghosh, S. S., Goulas, A., Falkiewicz, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Langs, G., … Smallwood, J. (2016). Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(44), 12574–12579. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608282113
McAdams, D. P. (2015). The art and science of personality development. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN: 9781462519958
McLean, K. C. (2016). The co-authored self: Family stories and the construction of personal identity. New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199995745.001.0001
Mendez, M. F., & Shapira, J. S. (2009). Altered emotional morality in frontotemporal dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14(3), 165–179. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800902924122
Molnar-Szakacs, I., & Uddin, L. Q. (2013). Self-processing and the default mode network: Interactions with the mirror neuron system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 571–571. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00571
Neubauer, S., Hublin, J.-J., & Gunz, P. (2018). The evolution of modern human brain shape. Science Advances, 4(1), eaao5961. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aao5961
Oatley, K. (2016). Fiction: Simulation of social worlds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 618–628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.002
Pace-Schott, E. F. (2013). Dreaming as a story-telling instinct. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 159. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00159
Pujol, J., Batalla, I., Contreras-Rodríguez, O., Harrison, B. J., Pera, V., Hernández-Ribas, R., … & Cardoner, N. (2012). Breakdown in the brain network subserving moral judgment in criminal psychopathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(8), 917–923. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr075
Reniers, R. L., Corcoran, R., Völlm, B. A., Mashru, A., Howard, R., & Liddle, P. F. (2012). Moral decision-making, ToM, empathy and the default mode network. Biological psychology, 90(3), 202–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.009
Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). Chapter one—The functional theory of counterfactual thinking: New evidence, new challenges, new insights. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 1–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2017.02.001
Schacter, D. L. (2018). Implicit memory, constructive memory, and imagining the future: A career perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(2), 256–272. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618803640
Schacter, D. L., Carpenter, A. C., Devitt, A., Roberts, R. P., & Addis, D. R. (2018). Constructive episodic simulation, flexible recombination, and memory errors. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, Article e32. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001510
Suddendorf, T. (2013). The gap: The science of what separates us from other animals. New York: Basic Books. ISBN: 9780465030149
Tamir, D. I., Bricker, A. B., Dodell-Feder, D., & Mitchell, J. P. (2015). Reading fiction and reading minds: The role of simulation in the default network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 215–224. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv114
Taylor, M. (2013). Imagination. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 791–831). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zhou, J., Greicius, M. D., Gennatas, E. D., Growdon, M. E., Jang, J. Y., Rabinovici, G. D., Kramer, J. H., Weiner, M., Miller, B. L., & Seeley, W. W. (2010). Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 133(5), 1352–1367. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq075